, 2004 and Guillaume et al , 2006) Presently, the only reported

, 2004 and Guillaume et al., 2006). Presently, the only reported and effective post-exposure therapy against Hendra or Nipah virus infection and one that could likely be approved in the near future for use in people has been a human monoclonal antibody (mAb) known as m102.4 which was isolated from a recombinant naïve human phage-displayed Fab library (Zhu et al., 2008). The m102.4 mAb has exceptionally potent neutralizing activity against both Nipah and Hendra viruses and its epitope maps to the ephrin receptor binding site (Fig. 1). Testing of m102.4 has confirmed its neutralization activity selleck inhibitor against several isolates; NiV-Malaysia, HeV-1994, HeV-Redlands, NiV-Bangladesh

(Bossart et al., 2009). Effective post-exposure efficacy with m102.4 has now been demonstrated in both ferrets and nonhuman primates (African green monkey (AGM)) infected with RAD001 clinical trial either Hendra virus or Nipah virus

(Table 1). The successful m102.4 passive immunotherapy in the AGM was recently reported in a study designed to reflect a possible real life scenario requiring mAb as a post-exposure treatment, and was a follow-up from the initial successful m102.4 post-exposure therapy carried out in ferrets (Bossart et al., 2009). Fourteen monkeys were challenged intratracheally with Hendra virus and 12 animals were infused twice with a 100 mg dose (∼20 mg/kg) of m102.4 beginning at 10 h, 24 h or 72 h p.i. with the second infusion ∼48 h later. All 12 animals that received m102.4 survived infection; whereas the untreated control subjects succumbed to severe systemic disease by day 8 (Bossart et al., 2011). There was no evidence of Hendra virus mediated pathology in any of the m102.4-treated animals and no infectious Hendra virus could be recovered from any tissues from any m102.4-treated subjects. In May of 2010,

an instance of possible Hendra Urocanase virus infection in two individuals was reported on the Sunshine Coast, north of Brisbane, Australia. Both individuals had extensive close contact with a horse just prior to and during the development of clinical illness in the animal. Following a diagnosis of Hendra virus infection in the horse, both individuals were considered to have had high-risk exposure to Hendra virus (Anonymous, 2010). A request was made by Australian health authorities to obtain m102.4 as a possible compassionate use therapeutic option even though clinical trials in human had not been undertaken and safety data of the mAb in humans was lacking. These two individuals were administered the m102.4 mAb (Miles, 2010). Both individuals ultimately did not develop detectable Hendra virus infection but whether this was due to the mAb therapy could not be determined. In 2010, the cell line expressing the human m102.4 mAb was provided to the Queensland Government, Queensland Health, to allow health authorities to manufacture m102.4 for its potential use on a compassionate basis in future cases of high-risk human exposure.

While a GRP modeling approach offers a more mechanistic means tha

While a GRP modeling approach offers a more mechanistic means than linear regression to estimate target nutrient loads, this approach is static, and hence, cannot account for the likely feedbacks and indirect effects that might exist as temperature and hypoxia vary through space and time. For example, behavioral avoidance of hypoxia has been shown to lead to highly dynamic predator–prey interactions

and density-dependent growth, and these changes in predator–prey interactions can cascade to not only affect a single predator–prey pair, but also the entire food web. Thus, we also have been exploring the effects of hypoxia and other habitat attributes (e.g., temperature, prey availability) on fish using more dynamic approaches, such as individual- and population-based bioenergetics simulations (individual-based Akt inhibitor modeling; D. Goto, personal communication), fish population behavior (patch-choice modeling; K. Pangle, personal communication), trophic interactions (Ecopath with Ecosim; e.g. Langseth et al., 2012), and comprehensive ecosystem responses (Comprehensive Aquatic Systems Modeling, CASM;

e.g. Bartell, 2003). These modeling approaches differ greatly in their spatial and temporal resolution and focus on the entire foodweb versus a subset of abundant, representative species. The differential emphasis on behaviorally mediated habitat selection, trophic interactions and trophic cascades among these models may lead to somewhat dissimilar predictions regarding ecological effects of hypoxia in Lake Erie. The integration INCB024360 solubility dmso Mirabegron of output from these diverse modeling approaches collectively provide a suite of plausible forecasts, as well as by help to identify key uncertainties that can guide future monitoring and research decisions. Because

of increases in hypoxia since the mid-1990s and because other eutrophication symptoms and potential impacts have become stronger since then, consideration of new phosphorus loading targets seems warranted. The use of models to assist in developing nutrient loading targets for the Great Lakes has a long history. Bierman (1980) reviewed their use as part of the negotiation of the earlier GLWQA, at which time five models were used to develop P loading objectives. The models ranged from simple, empirical correlations to complex mechanistic models (Bierman and Dolan, 1976, Bierman et al., 1980, Chapra, 1977, DiToro and Connolly, 1980, DiToro and Matystik, 1980, Hydroscience, 1976, Thomann et al., 1975, Thomann et al., 1976 and Vollenweider, 1977). Since that time, a variety of biogeochemical models have been developed to understand ecological interactions within Lake Erie and other Great Lakes. While some models were constructed during the 1980s (e.g., DePinto et al., 1986c, Di Toro et al., 1987, Lam et al., 1987a, Lam et al.

2 km upstream (Fig 2) A major flood occurred in 1913 shortly af

2 km upstream (Fig. 2). A major flood occurred in 1913 shortly after the construction of the dam. Although this flood did not damage the Gorge Dam, further upstream, the Le Fever Dam failed (Raub, Selleck Ruxolitinib 1984 and Whitman et al., 2010, p. 62, 64). The Northern Ohio Power and Light Company (later the Ohio Edison Company, and now First Energy Corporation) coal-fired power plant was in operation from 1912 to 1991 and was removed in 2009. When it began operation it produced 27,000 kW

of electricity and burned 91,000 tonnes of coal per year (Whitman et al., 2010, p. 80). The coal-fired power plant was enlarged and modified in 1930, 1940, and 1960. The Gorge Hydro Generating Station was in operation between 1915 and 1958 and was removed in 1977 (Whitman et al., 2010, p. 85). From 2005 to 2009, the Metro Parks, Serving

Summit County and Metro Hydroelectric Co. LLC were in legal proceedings regarding the construction of new hydroelectric facilities at the Gorge Dam (Vradenburg, 2012). The new construction plans have ended and currently the Ohio EPA is investigating removing both the dam pool sediment and the dam as a means of river restoration (Vradenburg, 2012). The removal of the Gorge Dam fits within a larger restoration effort of the Cuyahoga River in which the Munroe Falls and Kent Dams have already been removed (Tuckerman and Angiogenesis inhibitor Zawiski, 2007). About 23.2 km upstream from the Gorge Dam, the Lake Rockwell Dam was constructed in 1913 to provide water to the

City of Akron (U.S. Army Corps of Engineers, 2008). Thus, the Gorge Dam pool functions as a sediment trap of the 337 km2 Middle Cuyahoga Watershed but not the ZD1839 in vitro Upper Cuyahoga Watershed (Fig. 1). Within the Middle Cuyahoga watershed there are other small dams on the Cuyahoga River. Going upstream of the Gorge Dam, the Sheraton (2.6 km), Le Fever (3.1 km), Munroe Falls (8.5 km) and Kent (16.4 km) Dams were all in place before the Gorge Dam was constructed. The Le Fever and Munroe Falls Dams trapped fluvial sediment in the slack-water margins and had deep-water channels with little to no sediment accumulation (Peck et al., 2007 and Kasper, 2010). Hence, the Le Fever and Munroe Falls Dams allowed some sediment to travel farther downstream to the Gorge Dam pool. Because the Sheraton and Kent dam pools were confined to narrow bedrock channels with high velocity flows, they do not contain significant sediment deposits. In 2004 and 2005 the Kent Dam was altered to restore flow, and the Munroe Falls Dam was removed. Twelve modified-Livingstone piston cores were collected from the Gorge Dam pool in May and September, 2011 (Fig. 2). Nine of the 12 cores reached bedrock, and detailed information about each core and subsequent analyses can be found in Mann (2012). The cores are archived in the Department of Geosciences at the University of Akron.

They mature rapidly and provide the highest caloric meat yield of

They mature rapidly and provide the highest caloric meat yield of any of the available domesticates ( McClure et al., 2006). Since pigs are omnivorous

they can convert refuse and spoilage into a nutrient rich food source. On the other hand, pigs cannot convert cellulose-rich grasses into proteins, have higher water requirements, and do not tolerate heat well ( Zeder, 1996 and Zeder, 1998). The relative importance of pigs as a domesticate in early farming communities varied tremendously throughout Europe. In parts of the western Mediterranean pigs comprise the second largest percentage of domestic faunal remains at Neolithic archeological sites after ovicaprids AZD6244 concentration (e.g., Valencia Spain; Bernabeu, 1995, Hadjikoumis, 2011, McClure et al., 2006 and Pérez, 2002). In contrast, Neolithic sites in the Balkans tend to have few pig remains (Table 2). In addition to net increases in species and genetic biodiversity through animal introductions and interbreeding, individuals or at times groups of domesticated animals have reverted to living in

a wild or semi-wild state with little or no human management. Feralization likely began occurring at the onset of species introductions and its effects go beyond biological components of the animals. Indeed, Zeder (2012, p. 237) points out that some of the biological changes of domestication are irreversible, particularly brain size and function. One example is the wild mouflon (Ovis orientalis musimon), feralized descendants of domestic sheep on Mediterranean islands MK-2206 that retain the smaller brain size of their domestic ancestors despite looking like wild sheep ( Zeder, 2012; see also Groves, 1989 and Bruford and Townsend,

2006). In the case of feralization, the effects on biodiversity may well be best grasped as ecosystem biodiversity, where animals of a particular genetic makeup begin to inhabit new ecological niches independent of human control. In order to better grasp the implications of domesticated animals for ecosystem diversity, I turn to current paleoecological data for the region to assess the Baf-A1 degree of impact on a broader scale. The ecological impacts of introduced domesticates are difficult to discern for the earliest phases of the spread of agriculture. Modern analogies of domesticated grazers and browsers in new regions or studies of feral populations in island environments point to widespread and rapid decimation of vegetation coverage and resulting increases in erosion (e.g., Coblentz, 1978, Keegan et al., 1994 and Yocom, 1967). However, these examples tend to be large in scale, often dealing with situations where extensive numbers of animals are introduced, abandoned, or have escaped in contexts where predators are lacking and resource competition is depressed.

Key to the rise of later agricultural developments, growing human

Key to the rise of later agricultural developments, growing human numbers, and increasing social complexity was the intensive harvest collecting of acorns, walnuts, abundant seeds including annual grains and wild rice, and various roots, vegetables and fruits that people could gather in quantity

and store. Because agriculture was such a fundamental force in the development of all that followed, we pay particular attention to the evidence for its earliest beginnings and the socioeconomic developments it entrained. Pottery played an essential role in cooking, eating, and storing these highly varied plant foods. In considering its origins, it is important to note that some of the earliest known pottery vessels of East Asia bear imprints indicating that their originally pliable Caspase inhibitor wet clay was probably molded in tightly woven bags or baskets. Plaiting and weaving is a much older human art than

pottery-making, and the boiling of stews and soups by dropping hot stones from a fireplace into a liquid-filled woven bag or bark bucket is an ancient form of cookery that was still practiced in exigent situations during historical times in the circum-boreal zone. The early pottery of China, Korea, Japan, and the Russian Far East was a break-through invention of practical containers far more easily RO4929097 solubility dmso and cheaply made than the labor-intensive woven plant fiber prototypes that came before. It caught on rapidly all over East GBA3 Asia and was fundamental to the agricultural and social revolutions that were to follow. The invention of fired clay pottery as early as 18,000 cal BP provided a key tool for storing, cooking, and eating diverse foods made newly abundant by postglacial climatic change, and was instrumental in supporting human population growth

(Liu and Chen, 2012 and Zhushchikhovskaya, 2005). It caught on rapidly all over East Asia and was fundamental to the agricultural and social revolutions that were to follow. Thus, the abundant nuts and seeds and other foods increasingly available in the warming postglacial landscape of East Asia became a bonanza for human populations. Botanical research documents that many of the domesticated plants of East Asia descended from species that early people initially gathered as wild foods, or even as weeds that grew in the disturbed earth of human encampments (Aikens and Akazawa, 1996, Crawford, 1997, Crawford, 2006, Crawford, 2008, Crawford, 2011a, Crawford, 2011b, Crawford and Lee, 2003, Lee, 2011, Liu and Chen, 2012 and Tsukada et al., 1986).

51, t = 2 80; total time: b = 55 08, t = 2 21, go-past time: b = 

51, t = 2.80; total time: b = 55.08, t = 2.21, go-past time: b = 41.51, t = 2.20) with the exception of first fixation duration (b = 3.98, t = 0.60) and single fixation duration (b = 8.11, t = 0.98) whereas predictability was not modulated by task in any reading measure (all ts < 1.37) except for total time (b = 57.60, t = 2.72). These data suggest that, when checking for spelling errors that produce real but inappropriate words, proofreaders

still perform a qualitatively different type find more of word processing, which specifically amplifies effects of word frequency. However, while proofreaders do not appear to change their use of predictability during initial word recognition (i.e., first pass reading), later word processing does show increased effects of how well the word fits into the context of the sentence (i.e., during total time). We return to the issue of why this effect only appears on a late measure in Section 4.2. As with the reading time measures reported in Section 3.2.2.1, fixation probability measures showed a robust effect of task, with a higher probability of fixating the target (frequency items: z = 4.92, p < .001; predictability items: z = 5.41, p < .001), regressing into the target (frequency items: z = 5.60, p < .001; predictability items: z = 6.05, p < .001) and regressing out of the target (frequency items: z = 3.64, p < .001; predictability

items: z = 4.15, p < .001) in the proofreading task than in the reading task. Frequency yielded a main effect on probability of fixating the target (z = 5.77, p < .001) and probability of regressing out Pictilisib Calpain of the target (z = 2.56, p < .01) but not probability of regressing into the target (p > .15). Predictability yielded a marginal effect

on the probability of fixating the target (z = 1.77, p = .08) and a significant effect on the probability of regressing into the target (z = 5.35, p < .001) and regressing out of the target (z = 3.71, p < .001). There was a significant interaction between task and frequency on the probability of fixating the target (z = 2.14, p < .05) and a marginal interaction on the probability of regressing out of the target (z = 1.77, p = .08). All other interactions were not significant (all ps > .17). Thus, it seems as if the interactions seen in total time in Experiment 2 were not due to an increased likelihood of making a regression into or out of the target word, but rather to the amount of time spent on the word during rereading. As in Experiment 1, we tested for the three-way interaction between target type (frequency vs. predictability), independent variable value (high vs. low) and task (reading vs. proofreading) to evaluate whether the interactions between independent variable and task were different between the frequency stimuli and the predictability stimuli. As in Section 2.2.2.3, we tested for the three-way interaction in two key measures: gaze duration (Fig.

The MCE, combined with previous documentation of prehistoric Nati

The MCE, combined with previous documentation of prehistoric Native American events tied to farming and forest clearance (Stinchcomb et al., 2012), early Euro-American mill dam production and plowing of uplands (Walter and Merritts, 2008), and widespread

Mn aerosol deposition associated with industrial fallout (Herndon et al., 2011), demonstrate the spatial and temporal complexity of human impact on the stratigraphic record for the Northeastern USA. And thus, this study shows that anthropogenic impact on a regional scale is inherently complex and consists GPCR Compound Library clinical trial of a number of events. Although the MCE may not be a good candidate for a global Anthropocene boundary marker, it does provide researchers from various disciplines a more comprehensive picture of industrial-era coal production and its impact on riverine settings. Additional mapping and age-refinement of the MCE may provide local planners and policy makers with more information about Screening Library supplier history of land-use in the region. It could also help mitigate flood remobilization of preexisting MCE deposits that blanket much of the Lehigh, Schuylkill and North Branch Susquehanna River floodplains. Furthermore, the Anthropogenic Event method documented here provides a “ground-up” approach of documenting anthropogenic events on a local, regional, and global scale, which may be the necessary first step toward building an Anthropocene stratigraphy that

GABA Receptor provides value for geoscientists that can then be translated to the public. We would like to acknowledge the staff at Lehigh Gorge State Park for access to the Nesquehoning

Creek Site, Bureau for Historic Preservation of the Pennsylvania Historical and Museum Commission, the State Museum of Pennsylvania; Frank Vento of Clarion University, Peter Siegel of Montclair University, Ingrid Wuebber of the URS Corp., and Dan Wagner. We also thank Matt Harris for his efforts and insights into the presence of coal alluvial deposits along the Schuylkill River. We would like to thank Anne Jefferson, Karl Wegmann and Anne Chin for organizing the GSA 2012 special session, Geomorphology of the Anthropocene, which led to many fruitful discussions and helped propel the direction of this work. “
“One of the greatest modifications of the fluvial landscape in the Anthropocene is the construction of dams. Approximately 800,000 dams have been constructed worldwide (Gleick, 1998 and Friedl and Wuest, 2002). On a global scale, river damming has increased the mean residence time of river waters from 16 to 47 days and has increased the volume of standing water more than 700 percent (Friedl and Wuest, 2002). The timescale of major dam-building was contemporaneous globally, with an extreme acceleration in activity in 1950 and a peak in 1968 (Petts and Gurnell, 2005). More than 80,000 dams are currently in the United States with a quarter of these built in the 1960s (Graf, 2005).

, 2003) Most recorded sites were pointed out to researchers by l

, 2003). Most recorded sites were pointed out to researchers by locals (e.g., Nimuendaju, 2004). Though major phases of human occupation and environmental change have emerged from site research, most sites have not been investigated comprehensively, and there has been only limited coverage over Amazonia as a whole. Though only a tiny proportion of Amazonia has been examined, thousands of sites have been discovered in the diverse regions examined by researchers. As more areas are examined and more sites are found, new

regional cultures are being discovered (Fig. 1). Aerial survey was important in geographers’ early revelations about large wetland raised field systems (Denevan, 1966), but few sites of any kind have been mapped with instruments and even fewer with ground-probing geophysical technology (e.g., Bevan and Roosevelt, 2003, Roosevelt, 1991b and Roosevelt, UMI-77 2007). SCR7 datasheet Anthropic deposits that affect geomorphology over large areas are in principle detectable from the air or from space in many ways (e.g., El Baz and Wiseman, 2007). With such methods, we could better evaluate the patterning,

scope, and functioning of site complexes. Evidence of different cultures and land-management systems in Amazonia has come from stratigraphic analysis of sediments (e.g., Heckenberger, 2004, Iriarte et al., 2010, Morais and Neves, 2012, Neves, 2012, Piperno and Pearsall, 1998, Prumers,

2013, Roosevelt, 1991b, Roosevelt, 1997, Roosevelt et al., 1996, Rostain, 2010, Rostain, 2012 and Rostain, 2013). Excavation defines sites’ cultural components, layering, activity areas, and sequences of occupation. Soil processing to recover artifacts and ecofacts from strata gives evidence of specific past environments and economies and materials for dating. Where stratigraphy is not purposefully sampled, analyzed, and dated, questionable conclusions ensue, such as Pleistocene savannization and desertification (Whitmore and Prance, 1987) or megafaunal extinctions Thiamet G (Coltorti et al., 2012), unsupported by more comprehensive and critical studies (see Section ‘Environmental background’). And extrapolations not based on excavated cross-sections (van der Hammen and Absy, 1994:255, Fig. 2; Lombardo et al., 2013a, Fig. 2) do not accurately represent stratigraphy. Coring has been a main method for sampling offsite sediments to reconstruct past environments and land use. However, site formation processes and effectiveness of coring are seldom evaluated. Cores are often interpreted as direct evidence of regional climate change, without consideration of processes of local hydrology. For example, if an ancient water body dries up, this is interpreted as epochal climate change, though lake levels can change because of local hydrological or tectonic shifts (Colinvaux et al., 2000).

In the developing thalamus, as the number of retinogeniculate axo

In the developing thalamus, as the number of retinogeniculate axons innervating individual geniculocortical neurons drops, the synaptic strength of the remaining input rises (Chen and Regehr, 2000), perhaps due to elaboration of new synapses. In all these cases, axon loss could be associated with an increase in synapse number due to additional synaptogenesis from one of the remaining inputs. We think the same theme is also apparent at the neuromuscular junction despite confusion in nomenclature about the word “synapse.” Although the adult singly innervated neuromuscular junction is referred to as a “synapse,” it is actually a IWR-1 manufacturer cluster of synaptic release

sites. As muscle fibers grow, the postsynaptic area increases and the nerve terminal opposed to it enlarges, probably adding many new release sites (Marques et al., 2000). Recent data suggests that the reason the axons can elaborate new sites is that the presence of recently vacated postsynaptic sites causes nearby synaptic terminals to sprout to innervate the unoccupied acetylcholine receptors (Turney and Lichtman, 2012). Hence, postsynaptic sites are exchanged between axons, with the eliminated axons ceding their sites to the remaining ones and allowing the survivors to

increase their quantal content (Colman et al., 1997 and Walsh and Lichtman, 2003). Moreover, target muscle fibers and their postsynaptic territories continue to grow so the final result is that the axon that remains has 3 Methyladenine overall many more synaptic release sites than the ten or so axons that converged at birth. If an analogous synaptic exchange occurs in the developing brain, the rise in synapse number observed in the developing central nervous system might mask a loss of axonal inputs that is commensurate with the dramatic events occurring in developing muscle. An expanded Experimental Procedures section is provided in the Supplemental Experimental Procedures. Transgenic mice (Feng et al., 2000) were bred and housed according to the guidelines of the Harvard Animal Care and Use Committee. Pups were deeply anaesthetized with KX (ketamine/xylazine) and Protein tyrosine phosphatase transcardially

perfused with 2% paraformaldehyde. Isolated muscles were immunostained using a primary antibody to GFP (Chemicon) and a secondary antibody conjugated with Alexa 488 (Invitrogen). Motor units were imaged with confocal microscopy (Olympus FV-1000) using a 60× PlanAPO (1.4 NA) objective by excitation of Alexa 488 antibody and the Alexa 647-tagged α-bungarotoxin. Care was taken to magnify the images via laser scanning at the diffraction limit to assure that the finest processes were well resolved. Tissue for electron microscopy was processed as previously described (Hayworth et al., 2006 and Tapia et al., 2012). Sections were placed on a silicon wafer and imaged at ∼10kV (JEOL 6701F). All montages were aligned and segmented using TrakEM2 in NIH Image (Cardona et al., 2010).

, 2007; McDonald,

, 2007; McDonald, buy Tanespimycin 1998; Stefanacci and Amaral,

2002). However, it is less clear to what extent the amygdala alone can support complex forms of learning (Bryden et al., 2011; Holland and Gallagher, 2004; Li et al., 2011; Roesch et al., 2010; Vazdarjanova and McGaugh, 1998) and specifically probabilistic relationships as in partial reinforcement. The dACC has been implicated with monitoring of behavior, attention, signaling of error likelihood, and reinforcement volatility (Carter et al., 1998; Rushworth and Behrens, 2008; Wallis and Kennerley, 2010) and can therefore be more adept for learning complex relationships and contingencies. This is in line with our finding that activity in the dACC precedes the behavioral response at ParS, whereas neural

activity in the amygdala precedes behavior during ConS, although the behavioral response Smad inhibition itself was indistinguishable in both conditions. The synchronized discharge of both regions spiked at the beginning of learning but dropped back to baseline within a few trials of ConS. One option is that amygdala-dACC interactions are required for the initial learning phase, but not for the maintenance of the memory once it is formed and synaptic changes are made downstream. Another option is that the dACC takes an active part by default but then lowers its communication with the amygdala when it realizes that it is not required for the simple associations. This can be achieved by feedback reports about correct behavior. In sharp contrast to ConS, the amygdala-dACC synchronized activity maintained during ParS, even much after behavioral plateau was obtained and was similar for ConS and ParS (trials 4–30). This finding suggests that these correlations are required for active maintenance

of the memory under ParS. This is further supported by the fact that the magnitude of these correlations at the end of learning, and their locking to CS, were a reliable predictor for the difficulty (length) of the following extinction training. Why should amygdala-dACC correlations make the memory harder to extinguish? Extinction is a new learning that was shown to be mediated by subregions of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). This includes the rodent infralimbic cortex (IL) (Milad and Quirk, 2002; Sierra-Mercado click here et al., 2011) and the primate vmPFC (Phelps et al., 2004). These regions exhibit opposite activation patterns to that of the amygdala and are activated during extinction recall, whereas the amygdala is inhibited. The primate dACC was shown to have the opposite effect on fear expression and extinction (Dunsmoor et al., 2007b; Milad et al., 2007), similar to the rodent prelimbic cortex (PL) (Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011; Vidal-Gonzalez et al., 2006), and promotes fear in general (Burgos-Robles et al., 2009). Hence, these are probably two competing pathways with opposite effects.